lemÌc. Lemizh grammar and dictionary

Unit 12. Time and space

ΔτÂČ = Δx^ÎŒ Δx^Îœ η_ΌΜ = ΔtÂČ âˆ’ (ΔxÂČ + ΔyÂČ + ΔzÂČ) / cÂČ

(Hendrik Antoon Lorentz)

Temporal and spatial cases

Have another look at the last two columns of the table of case descriptors.

As we have seen in unit 3, the spatial cases numbered 1 to 4 (expressing place, starting point, extent, end point) are action-centred: if I speak to you in a spaceship-loc, the whole action (which starts from me and reaches you) is in the spaceship. To express that only I am in the spaceship, we need a spatial attribute, which is treated further down in this unit. Cases 5 to 8 (expressing scene, place away from which, crossing point, aim) are agent-centred. This is obvious with the allative case, which expresses a spatial aim, but is also true of the others: the ablative names the place the agent is fleeing or the, well, spatial reason for now being here, in much the same sense as the allative names the spatial purpose of his journey. The scenic names the scene or ‘stage’ the agent has chosen for the action. The prolative names the place the agent intentionally comes across, as in ‘jump through a hoop’ or ‘cross the street’. — The temporal cases work analogously.

The action-centred cases have to be used with care – if we say we are going to Mars-ill, this would mean we’d end up on the whole planet. Working alternatives, and their limitations, are:

No such measures are necessary for points within continuous things, thanks to Rule Six of sentence grammar. (See the genitives ‘A Midsummer Night’s Dream’ and ‘men of Lemaria’ in unit 5.)

The names for the case pairs elative/illative and ingressive/egressive are somewhat inconsistent, but the first two are established names (in Finno-Ugric grammar), and the others are termed rather obviously; in other words: I couldn’t think of a more logical terminology. Native Lemizh grammar has more symmetric names for these cases.

Temporal and spatial verbs

A beaver as the reference object of a coordinate system. The first axis points from tail to head, the second upwards, and the third from left to right. ‘far’ is somewhere in the distance, ‘ouside’ outside the beaver’s body, and ‘between’ is between its paws.

Temporal and spatial verbs are adjective-like words denoting actions like ‘to make points / an area in a region X; to turn into points / an area in a region X’.

The first three spatial verbs, the axis verbs, correspond to the positive sides of the three axes of a Cartesian coordinate system that has its origin at, and is oriented along, a reference object, which can be a living being, a thing, a movement or other action – whatever has an orientation in space. The first axis (front) points in the direction of the ‘face’ or interacting side of the reference object. For oriented actions, this axis is parallel to the spatial arrow. The second axis (up) points upwards, in the direction of the sky, if the reference object is in its ordinary position. The third axis (right) forms a right-handed coordinate system with the first and second. Some reference objects only define the first axis (an arrow, for example), others only the second (a free-standing tree). Obviously, the third axis is only defined if the first two are.

The reference object is the nominative object of the spatial verb. (See the kinship verbs in unit 5 for context.) The coordinate system is aligned with it: if I lie down on my back, my front-axis points towards the sky, and my up-axis is level with the ground. The ‘right’ side of a wardrobe is what we would call its left because its interacting side faces you if you are standing in front of it; so the axes are arranged as if a human were facing you. The reference object restricts the set of possible coordinate systems, but not necessarily to a single one: a winding river has many possibilities for the coordinate system’s origin and associated orientation. Most of the time, the resulting subset is still useful: the meaning of an object being on the river’s right side is clear.

As there is only one axis in time, we only need the first axis (the front-axis) to convey temporal information. This axis has its origin at a reference object that is often, but not necessarily, an action. (It could also be a soap bubble or even a human.) It points in the direction of the temporal arrow, towards the future.

The three remaining verbs are ‘far’, ‘outside’ and ‘between’. These serve as both temporal and spatial verbs.


Here is an overview of the temporal and spatial verbs with an inner accusative and the relevant outer cases, including compounds with the weighting numeral crÌ. ‘a bit’ and the opposition negator kÌ., which weaken or negate the front-ness, up-ness, etc., respectively (abstract nouns, hence with an epenthetic consecutive). The meanings with other outer cases are easily derived from the temporal and locative rows; for example, the elative (starting point) object — Ìfer. 
 up-acc-ela2. means ‘from above, down’; the illative (end point) object — ilfkÌir. 
 up-cons-opposition-acc-ill2. also means ‘down’, but in the sense of ‘to a place below’.

VerbprĂ .Ă f.rĂ c.fĂ w.gmĂ .nĂ j.
Glossfrontuprightfaroutsidebetween
Translation
with inner accusative
and outer â€Š
Temporal
(aR)
Translationlater, afterat a distant timebefore or afterbetween (times)
crÌ.a bit laterabout nowjust before or afterjust between (times)
kÌ.earlier, before—during, whilebefore or after (times)
Durative
(yR)
Translationlong (duration)*far reaching, extensive (time span)*——
crÌ.short (duration)*with a small extent (time span)*——
kÌ.————
Locative
(ar)
Translationin frontaboveat the rightfar away, (over) thereoutsidebetween
crÌ.just in front, etc.nearby, herejust outsidejust between
kÌ.at the backbelowat the left—insideoutside (a group of things)
Extensive
(yr)
Translationlong; deep (wardrobe)high, tall; deep (pond)broad, widefar reaching, extensive*——
crÌ.short; shallow (wardrobe)low, small in height;
shallow (pond)
narrowwith a small extent*——
kÌ.——————

* prÌ. ‘front’ in the durative and fÌw. ‘far’ in the durative and extensive cases are similar in meaning to weighting numerals; the difference is that the former have a broader, less specific, meaning.

Illustration of up-ness. Explanation see text. Illustration of far-ness. Illustration of outside-ness. Illustration of between-ness.

These rather psychedelic diagrams illustrate the concepts of up-ness (a), far-ness (b), outside-ness (c), and between-ness (d). The reference objects are the black dots and circle; green areas are considered to be up, far, outside and between, respectively; magenta areas are the opposites; and the darkness of the colours represents the weight or intensity. Front-ness and right-ness are symmetric to diagram (a), only with the arrow pointing in other directions.

(a) shows that the notion of ‘up’ is a combination of vertical distance and angle: ilfjnÌ. up-cons-1/1-acc1. is high up, exactly above; lesser weights are shifted sideways and/or lower down. ilfnÌ. up-cons-not-acc1. is on one level with the reference object. The difference between (b) and (c) is that the outside begins at the surface of the reference object. Thus, we can describe a large area as extending far away, but not as extending to its outside. Between-ness (d) is far-ness from parts of a reference object (which typically consists of several things, or a concave thing) in the direction of other parts.

The time spans and areas denoted by temporal and spatial verbs are continuous things in the above sense, so we don’t need a fragmenting partitive to describe something that occupies only part of the space in front.

As an aside: these verbs can also be used with causal cases, the front-axis then being aligned with the causal arrow: — prÌal. 
 front-acc-aff2. ‘causally downstream’, — fÌwal. 
 far-acc-aff2. ‘with a distant causal connection’, etc.

Durative and extensive

The durative’s and extensive’s case descriptors, ‘duration’ and ‘extent’, respectively, should be understood literally: these cases cannot denote specific points or regions in time or space. Thus, ‘extending to the outside’ is not translated as **— gmÌyr. 
 outside-acc-ext2. but as — gmÌnar. 
 outside-partacc-loc2. ‘outside, among other places’. The same goes for ‘extending to the front of the ship’.

One consequence is that it is hardly ever necessary to name the reference object of a durative or extensive construction, it usually being identical to the described thing. But even — prÌyr Ìkhe. 
 front-acc-ext2 ship-acc-nom3. means ‘extending parallel to the ship’s first axis’ (and not ‘extending to the front of the ship’).

Pronouns

The meaning of pronouns distinguishing distance, as ‘this [here]’ vs. ‘that [there]’, can be conveyed with spatial verbs such as filwcrÌ. far-cons-1/4-acc1. ‘something near’ and fÌw. far-acc1. ‘something far’ (or with other weighting numerals), as well as the more specific Ìf. up-acc1. ‘something up [there]’, filwcrÌ vìe. far-cons-1/4-acc1 PIn−2-dat-nom2. ‘something near you’ (with reference object), etc.

Adverbials and adjectivals

Adverbials

Adverbials (adverbs and adverbial phrases) containing temporal or spatial information about actions are simply translated as objects in temporal or spatial cases, which often but not always contain a temporal or spatial verb.

qáxk ykhÌ prÌar.The ship is flying at the front.
fly-fact1 ship-acc-acc2a front-acc-loc2.
jàx qnytÌ Ìkher.The smoke comes from the ship.
move-fact1 smoke-acc-acc2 ship-acc-ela2.
jàx qnytÌ gmilkÌer Ìkhe.(with reference object)The smoke comes from the inside of the ship / from inside the ship.
move-fact1 smoke-acc-acc2 outside-cons-opposition-acc-ela2 ship-acc-nom3.
nÌj kroblÌje.the area between [the parts of] the castlethe coutyard of the castle
between-acc1 castle-acc-nom2.
lák stryÌ ilfjnÌur ihkùe.The cow jumps across an area exactly above the Moon.The cow jumps over the Moon.
jump-fact1 cow-acc-acc2a up-cons-1/1-acc-prol2 moon-nom-nom3.
dnà dmyÌR ÌkhOr.He walked for a long time away from the ship.
walk-fact1 3/4-acc-dur2 ship-acc-abl2.

Adjectivals and ‘inside’ constructions

Adjectivals (adjectives and adjectival phrases) describing properties and states are similar to predicate adjectives (‘She is beautiful’): they need topicalisation of the consecutive case; they are constructed with the accusative for depictive and with the dative for resultative meaning; and depictive ones can often be simplified by absorption.

‘in’ and ‘into’ often have to be translated with gmilkÌ. outside-cons-opposition-acc1. ‘inside’ or with a fragmenting partitive, plus locative or illative, respectively. As described above, a simple spatial object is only appropriate if the location is either continuous or filled completely, or if an agent-centered case (scenic or allative, respectively) can be used. ‘on’ and ‘onto’ work with gmilnÌ. outside-cons-not-acc1. ‘surface’.

mìl prygÌ gmilkÌar mÌge.The tower has been made in the space dock. (depictive)The tower-acc is in(side) the space dock. [It has always been there.]
make-cons1 tower-acc-acc2 outside-cons-opposition-acc-loc2 shipyard-acc-nom3.
→ prìlg gmilkÌar mÌge.(absorption)
tower-cons1 outside-cons-opposition-acc-loc2 shipyard-acc-nom3.
mìl prygÌ mÌgor. → prìlg mÌgor.(depictive, with scenic case)The tower-acc is in(side) the space dock. [They decided to build it there.]
make-cons1 tower-acc-acc2 shipyard-acc-sce2. → tower-cons1 shipyard-acc-sce2.
mìl ykhì mÌgor.The ship has become something in the space dock. (resultative, with scenic)The ship-dat is in(side) the space dock. [It has come there for maintenance.]
make-cons1 ship-acc-dat2 shipyard-acc-sce2.
mìl tnedwì dmyÌR ilfcrÌyr.The captain is old and small.
make-cons1 captain-nom-dat2 3/4-acc-dur2 up-cons-1/4-acc-ext2.
mìl prildmÌaR.It is late.
make-cons1 front-cons-3/4-acc-temp2.

Constructions such as mà gmilkÌar mÌge. make-fact1 outside-cons-opposition-acc-loc2 shipyard-acc-nom3. ‘become something in(side) the dock, be made into something in(side) the dock’ are pseudo-desorptions: this one would be a desorption if ‘in the dock’ were a property like ‘beautiful’ (i.e. an adjectival verb with with inner and outer accusatives).

Attributes

Temporal and spatial attributes are used to specify the time or place of a single object, rather than that of the whole plot. They can be phrased straightforwardly using the adverbial and adjectival constructions above; for adverbial and unabsorbed adjectival ones (i.e. those with the main predicate mĂ .), we need an inversion forming a bracket to get the object to the front.

An alternative to simple local attributes, especially with perceptual verbs, is an elative object of the main predicate; this moves the local information nearer to the predicate.

dmàt Ìkhe qÌxky prÌar.I see the ship flying at the front.
see-fact1 ship-acc-nom2 fly-acc-acc3 front-acc-loc4.
dmàt Ìkhe cryÌr mìy mÌgor.(resultative, with scenic)I see the small ship in the space dock.
see-fact1 ship-acc-nom2 1/4-acc-ext3 make-dat-acc3 shipyard-acc-sce4.
dmàt prÌge gmilkÌar mÌge.(depictive, absorption)I see the tower in the space dock.
see-fact1 tower-acc-nom2 outside-cons-opposition-acc-loc3 shipyard-acc-nom4.
dmàt mìe rycàr Ìkhin.I see the right one of the ships, the ship at the right.
see-fact1 make-dat-nom2 right-acc-loc3 ship-acc-partdat3.
dmàt prygù gmilkÌer mÌge.(The optical stimulus comes from inside the space dock.)I see the tower in the space dock.
see-fact1 tower-acc-nom2 outside-cons-opposition-acc-ela2 shipyard-acc-nom3.

Measuring

Temporal and spatial verbs can also be specified with units of measurement, which are basically numerals with a physical dimension; and they used as abstract nouns like the exponential numbers. This of course means we multiply using the consecutive case. For more on units of measurement, see the appendix.

trĂŹlxk prÌyr frÌil xrĂŹly. ⇒ trĂŹlxk prÌyr xrilfrÌil.The front-ness is 12 length units (of 9.2 cm each).The beaver is 110 cm (43Âœ in) long.
beaver-cons1 front-acc-ext2 twelve-acc-cons3 lengthunit-cons-acc4. ⇒ beaver-cons1 front-acc-ext2 lengthunit-cons-twelve-acc-cons3.
blàdj prilkÌaR djilvfdwÌil.two weeks’ notice
announce-fact1 front-cons-opposition-acc-temp2 week-cons-two-acc-cons3.

Units in combination with axis verbs also distinguish horizontal or vertical distance from angle.

— ilfxrildmÌar.many length units upat a great height

 up-cons-lengthunit-cons-3/4-acc-loc2.
— ilfselildmÌar.many angle units upnear the zenith

 up-cons-angleunit-cons-3/4-acc-loc2.

Orientation

Orientation with respect to some object is expressed by stating that the object has a certain position in the oriented thing’s (or action’s) coordinate system. Obviously, resultative meaning requires the axis verb to be in the dative.

mĂ  prÌi dryzdĂ© jmÌsar.An area in front of the chair is made to be at the door.I turn the chair towards the door.
make-fact1 front-acc-dat2 chair-acc-nom3 door-acc-loc2.
mĂŹl prÌi dryzdĂ© jmÌsar.(perfect)The chair faces the door.
make-cons1 front-acc-dat2 chair-acc-nom3 door-acc-loc2.
prilnìl drÌe.The tree has no front-ness.The tree is symmetric; it is a free-standing tree.
front-cons-not-cons1 tree-acc-nom2.
khnà viÙr prilkÌer {nenáe cìy}.I am shouting towards you from the back (of your running).I am shouting after you.
shout-fact1 PIn−2-dat-all2 front-cons-opposition-acc-ela2 {run-fact-nom3 PIn−4-dat-acc4a}.

Some situations require two coordinate systems, with the advantage that we can use ‘classical’ predicative constructions with dative or accusative objects instead of spatial cases.

ìlf dryù ilfdmÌy.An area up in the tree’s coordinate system is an area much up in the general coordinate system. (depictive)The tree is leaning somewhat.
up-cons1 tree-acc-nom2 up-cons-3/4-acc-acc2.
prà lakù Ìfi fÌe.The area up of the jumper (pronoun) is made something at the front of the jump. (resultative)
front-fact1 jump-fact-nom2 up-acc-dat2 PIIn−1-acc-nom3.
⇔ làk prùa Ìfi zÌe.He jumps head first.
jump-fact1 front-nom-fact2 up-acc-dat3 PIn−3-acc-nom4.

Comparison

Statements we would express as comparisons can often be phrased with a reference object. Qualitative objects are only needed if there is no reference object we can use.

Compare:
mìl ykhì ilfnÌar mÌge.The ship is not above (i.e. at the same height as) the space dock.The ship is as high up as the space dock [in the space dock’s coordinate system].
make-cons1 ship-acc-dat2 up-cons-not-acc-loc2 shipyard-acc-nom3.
mìl ykhì Ìfar mÌge.The ship is above the space dock.The ship is higher up than the space dock [in the space dock’s coordinate system].
make-cons1 ship-acc-dat2 up-acc-loc2 shipyard-acc-nom3.
mìl ykhì mygìm Ìfar.The ship is as high up as the space dock [in another coordinate system, e.g. mine].
make-cons1 ship-acc-dat2 shipyard-acc-qualdat2 up-acc-loc2.
mìl ykhì mygìm ilftÌcdar.The ship is higher up than the space dock [in another coordinate system].
make-cons1 ship-acc-dat2 shipyard-acc-qualdat2 up-cons-more-acc-loc2.
àv prÌaR (váme cìim).(inner qualitative referring to ‘eat’)I’ll eat later (than you).
eat-fact1 front-acc-temp2 (PIn−2-qualfact-nom3 PIn−4-dat-qualdat4a).
àv viìm priltÌcdaR.(simpler phrasing; see ‘Romeo climbs more nimbly than a squirrel’)I’ll eat later than you.
eat-fact1 PIn−2-dat-qualdat2 front-cons-more-acc-temp2.

‘The stone is in front of the tree’ (with neither the tree nor the stone having a front axis) can be expressed along the same lines.

mìl dryì zmyjìm priltÌcdar.The tree is more in front [of me] than the stone.The stone is in front of the tree. The tree is behind the stone.
make-cons1 tree-acc-dat2 stone-acc-qualdat2 front-cons-more-acc-loc2.

The comparative verb tàcd. can be specified with a measurement in the consecutive case, typically giving a bracket. This construction is equivalent to ‘a bit faster’ but does not lend itself to compounding.

tĂŹlcd fĂ ty yhwĂš wygwĂ©m kilxÌjil.The more-ness is 16 speed units. The speed is more by 16 units.The horse is faster than the dog by 4 km⁄h (2Âœ mph).
more-cons1 fast-fact-acc2 horse-acc-nom3 dog-acc-qualnom3 speedunit-cons-16-acc-cons2.

Tense

The time of an action, what many languages express as tense, can be conveyed by marking another action (often the parole) as a reference object. Simultaneous action (present tense) is simply expressed by a pronoun as a temporal object. Both constructions can be compounded to downtone the temporal information.

dràw vàaR. ⇔ ⇒ draRwwà.The time of dancing is the parole.I am dancing [now].
dance-fact1 PIn−2-fact-temp2. ⇔ ⇒ dance-temp-PIn−1-fact1.
dràw prÌaR {zàe}. ⇔ ⇒ draRwprà.I will dance [later].
dance-fact1 front-acc-temp2 {PIn−3-fact-nom3}. ⇔ ⇒ dance-temp-front-fact1.
dràw prilkÌaR. ⇔ ⇒ draRwprilkà.I danced [earlier].
dance-fact1 front-cons-opposition-acc-temp2. ⇔ ⇒ dance-temp-front-cons-opposition-fact1.

Many English present tense forms do not express present actions. ‘I dance’ is typically understood as a general statement and translated either simply with dràw. dance-fact1. or with one of the aspects described in the next chapter to explicitly mark it as an iterative (repeated) or habitual action. The future tense frequently expresses an intention, opportunity or wish and so wants a topicalised tentive case or a modal verb for translation instead of a temporal object; I intend to treat modal verbs in the next unit. The past tense can often be translated simply as a perfect (with topicalised consecutive).

là ilvà prilneytfÌaR.(see Objects of topicalised verbs)I will have eaten by midnight.
do-fact1 eat-cons-fact2 midnight-acc-temp2.

Furthermore, as has been mentioned in other contexts, tense markers can and should be omitted whenever possible. A story, for example, only needs temporal objects in the first sentence and when the chronological flow is broken by flashbacks or forecasts, and often not even then. ‘tomorrow’ or ‘an hour ago’ already express the future or past, respectively; so adding one of the above temporal objects would be redundant.

The active participle can also be marked for tense in Lemizh. As with the passive participle, this is rarely necessary.

Ìx waRxvùy.a now speaking man; a man speaking at the moment
male-acc1 speak-temp-PIn−2-nom-acc2.
Ìx waRxprùy.a man who will be speaking
male-acc1 speak-temp-front-nom-acc2.

Aspect

Aspect is a grammatical category describing the temporal flow, in a broad sense, of an acton: once or habitually, short-lived or extended, completed or continuing, etc. Lemizh often uses numerals for these purposes, but also the verb nàjw. ‘pursue habitually’ (akin to xwà. ‘pursue a hobby’: ‘habit’ is a gerund-like abstract noun). The progressive aspect, which expresses an ongoing action, needs an inverted ‘inside’ construction. The gnomic aspect, expressing a general truth, translates as a depictive predicative.

nagwrà bunyÌ wÌcgy.(semelfactive: multiplicative numeral)I’ll drink my coffee black [this once].
drink-fact-one-fact1 coffee-acc-acc2 black-acc-acc2.
nagwmlà bunyÌ wÌcgy.(iterative)I drink my coffee black [at several occasions].
drink-fact-several-fact1 coffee-acc-acc2 black-acc-acc2.
nagwRà bunÌy rÌy.(distributive: each of the actions of drinking one coffee)I’m drinking my coffees one by one / one at a time.
drink-fact-each-fact1 coffee-acc-acc2 one-acc-acc3.
nagwnàjw bunyÌ wÌcgy.(habitual)I drink my coffee black [habitually].
drink-fact-habit-fact1 coffee-acc-acc2 black-acc-acc2.
nagwnàjw bunyÌ wycgÌ prilkÌaR.(habitual past)I would drink my coffee black [habitually] back then.
I used to drink my coffee black.
drink-fact-habit-fact1 coffee-acc-acc2 black-acc-acc2 front-cons-opposition-acc-temp2.
wà gmilkÌaR nàRgwe.(progressive)
PIn−1-fact1 outside-cons-opposition-acc-temp2 drink-temp-nom3.
⇔ nàgw bunyÌ wycgÌ gmilkùaR {zàRy}.I am drinking my coffee black [a currently ongoing event].
drink-fact1 coffee-acc-acc2 black-acc-acc2 outside-cons-opposition-nom-temp2 {PIn−3-temp-acc3}.
míl veÌ nìgwy bunyÌ wÌcgy.(gnomic: I am a black-coffee-drinker.)I drink my coffee black. [Full stop.]
make-cons1 PIn−2-nom-acc2a drink-dat-acc2 coffee-acc-acc3 black-acc-acc3.

The aspect opposite to the gnomic, the episodic, is represented by specifying the time of the action, or with a semelfactive aspect. The momentane (short-lived) and durative (extended) aspects use weighting numerals in the durative case. The perfect, as well as beginning and ending of actions, are translated by topicalising the main predicate. Strengthening and weakening of verbs (augmentative and diminutive) make use of weighting numerals. The completive aspect uses the numeral jnà. ‘all, the whole’, as in ‘eat up’, among other constructions such as an illative object with verbs of movement; the incompletive aspect accordingly uses weaker weighting numerals or a negated final topic.

The perfective, describing an action as a simple whole, is usually best left unmarked; sometimes it can be translated like the completive. The imperfective is a cover term for aspects describing actions with an inner structure, such as the iterative, the habitual, or the progressive.

Exercises

Translate:
The ship will fly in.Solve
The children were playing under the table just now.Solve
We eat in the hotel [and that’s a law of nature].
(three possibilities; which is the best here?)
Solve
It has rained for two days vs. It has been raining for two days.Solve
I am running towards the tree.
(two possibilities, one making use of the first axis verb)
Solve
the finger pointing upwards, the upward-pointing finger
(‘finger’ is a tool noun.)
Solve
Those houses a long way off are spread out over a large area.Solve
We are celebrating in anticipation of midnight.Solve
Under what circumstances would somebody phrase ‘men of Lemaria’ as Ìx gmilkÌar lemÌce.?Solve